Science lesson: smoking cessation is not medical treatment

The previous science lesson explained why clinical trials are not useful for assessing the effectiveness of vaping for quitting smoking. While clinical trials (medical experiments) are often a useful study method, they do not replicate the real-world experience of switching to vaping. Clinical trials can measure the effect of assigning vaping to random smokers. But we really want to know how helpful vaping is for smokers who choose to try it. How do so many people confuse these obviously very different questions?

Here’s the problem: Those who conduct such studies do not realize they are implicitly assuming everything is as simple as medical treatment. Anything more complicated than that is ignored. This mistake also causes other errors.

Clinical trials are good for determining which medical treatment is better. If Drug X and Drug Y are each pretty good treatments for a disease, it is useful to know which one works better on average. Usually patients should not take X and Y at the same time, so one must be chosen. Thus, figuring out the average effectiveness of each drug, when given to everyone with the disease, lets us identify which to assign to everyone. It may be that X would work better for some patients and Y for others, but we typically do not know in advance who is who, so need to go with the averages.

For this simple scenario, clinical trials show which works best on average, and they replicate the real-world situation (clinicians assigning a particular drug to patients with the disease). Unfortunately, this scenario creates a simplistic mindset that all health interventions are just easy to analyze. This results in nonsensical claims when the greater complexities of social science are ignored.

One problem is insisting that randomized trials are necessary before assessing the effectiveness of a particular intervention, like recommending vaping. Sometimes experiments are informative though certainly not necessary (if Drug X works ten times as often as Drug Y, casual observation will make the superiority of X clear). In other cases – such as with switching to vaping – trials are not even very informative.

Other problems are more subtle: It is wrong to claim that a study shows that vaping (or unaided quitting, or some other method) is the “most effective” way to quit smoking.

Some such claims are based on counting. Most studies find that the majority of smoking cessation is “unaided,” done without any substitute product, drugs, or formal intervention. But this tells us nothing about what method is most helpful. People who choose to quit smoking unaided believe that they are ready to just stop. They are often wrong, of course, but if someone believes that, it is reasonably likely to be true. If she does not believe it, she is almost certainly correct. Thus if someone is not inclined to try to quit unaided, but instead tried to switch to vaping or another substitute, it would be insane to tell her, “No, stop! Most people quit unaided, so just try that.”

More often, claims of “most effective” are based on the rate of success among people who tried a particular method. This is the medical treatment model of smoking cessation, and is equally wrong. Consider a study that compares the rates of success, in a particular population at a particular time.

The first thing to note is that last phrase is critically important. For medical treatments, human biology does not vary all that much, so if a clinical trial shows Drug X works better, there is a reasonable chance this is true for all humans. But for consumer choice – and everything else in social science – there are many differences across populations. Prayer might aid smoking cessation in some populations, but it is easy to predict it would have no effect in others. Trying to switch to vaping or another product is not going to work so well in a population where supply of the products is likely to be interrupted.

What if the hypothetical study found that those who tried to switch to vaping did not quit smoking as often than those trying unaided quitting? A naive interpretation would be that vaping does not help, or even hinders, smoking cessation. This is, of course, exactly the interpretation anti-vaping activists have offered for some such results. But for most populations for most of the time vaping has existed, switching has not been a common approach. Smokers who tried vaping had usually already considered – and tried – quitting unaided and with various government-recommended methods. They had a much harder time quitting than average, and so still did when they tried vaping.

Read more at dailyvaper.com

Advertisements

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out /  Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s